Cortex on Lithic Debitage
Chronological signatures for lithic wastes would be particularly useful because of the ubiquity and relative abundance of debitage in most prehistoric assemblages. For many sites in San Diego County, lithic wastes are the only evidence available for interpretation or for making comparisons with other, more diverse assemblages.
Martin D. Rosen (1987a:80-89) proposed that a high frequency of cortex on debitage may be diagnostic of Archaic (La Jolla) assemblages, as contrasted with Late Prehistoric assemblages. Rosen reported that an average of 30% of the debitage from a sample of Archaic assemblages contained cortex, while only 13% of the debitage from Late Prehistoric assemblages had cortex. Data from additional sites provide some support for Rosen’s generalization, although considerable overlap in cortex percentages between the two periods is also apparent.
Site | Main Period |
Geologic Setting |
Sample Size |
Percent with Cortex |
References |
SDI-197 | Archaic | coastal | 1,265 | 31 | Rosen 1987a |
SDI-1103 | Archaic | coastal | 305 | 31 | Laylander 1986a |
SDI-4615 | Archaic | coastal | 239 | 28 | Laylander 1986a |
SDI-6087 | Archaic | batholith | 2,311 | 10 | Cheever and Gallegos 1986 |
SDI-7197 | Archaic | coastal | 178 | 21 | Padon and Muranaka 1983 |
SDI-7197 | Archaic | coastal | 4,788 | 11 | Pigniolo and Gallegos 1990a |
SDI-9243 | Archaic | batholith | 17,065 | 13 | Corum and White 1986* |
SDI-9678 | Archaic | coastal | 1,117 | 28 | Rosen 1989 |
SDI-10,148 | Archaic | batholith | 312 | 21 | Carrico et al. 1991 |
SDI-11,570 | Archaic | volcanic | 297 | 18 | Pigniolo and Gallegos 1990b |
SDM-W-110 | Archaic | coastal | 1,602 | 13 | Walker and Bull 1981* |
SDM-W-1439 | Archaic | coastal | 2,103 | 26 | Hector 1984b |
La Costa | Archaic | volcanic | 904 | 30 | Hector 1985b* |
SDI-674 | Late | batholith | 1,281 | 12 | Rosen 1984* |
SDI-4513 | Late | coastal | 1,936 | 6 | Eidsness et al. 1979* |
SDI-4513 | Late | coastal | 730 | 8 | Rosen 1987b* |
SDI-4513 | Late | coastal | 3,831 | 25 | Hector and Wade 1986* |
SDI-4513 | Late | coastal | 678 | 32 | Gallegos et al. 1989 |
SDI-4609 | Late | coastal | 6,198 | 19 | Hector 1985a* |
SDI-4763 | Late | volcanic | 891 | 13 | Rosen 1982* |
SDI-5066 | Late | volcanic | 886 | 18 | Rosen 1982* |
SDI-5383 | Late | coastal | 6,867 | 14 | Laylander 1989 |
SDI-5680 | Late | volcanic | 18,496 | 14 | Dominici 1985* |
SDI-7306 | Late | volcanic | 491 | 11 | Pigniolo and Gallegos 1990b |
SDI-8534 | Late | batholith | 484 | 13 | Laylander 1986b |
SDI-8594A | Late | batholith | 270 | 11 | Carrico et al. 1991 |
SDI-9476 | Late | volcanic | 10,005 | 19 | Hector 1984a* |
SDI-11,626 | Late | coastal | 1,736 | 18 | Pigniolo and Gallegos 1990a |
* Cortex percentage as reported by Rosen 1987a.
Geologic Settings: coastal = sedimentary rocks of the coastal plain; volcanic = foothill/valley belt of Santiago Peak metavolcanics; batholith = valley/mountain belt of granitic and metamorphic rocks.
The presence or absence of cortex on lithic wastes may reflect the stage at which the wastes were produced within a lithic reduction sequence. One commonly applied classification, which distinguishes primary flakes with full dorsal cortex, secondary flakes with partial dorsal cortex, and tertiary flakes lacking cortex — implies such a sequence. The implication was made explicit in a nine-type lithic waste classification employed by Susan M. Hector (1984a), Rosen (1984), and others. In this scheme, small flakes and shatter that have cortex were assigned primarily to core preparation, initial reduction, and primary trimming activities, whereas small items that lack cortex were assigned to secondary trimming, shaping, maintenance, and modification activities.
Several possible explanations may be suggested for contrasts between different assemblages in the frequency of cortex:
- The contrasts may reflect differences in the end-products intended by the knappers. Specifically, Archaic-period knapping may have been concerned to a large extent with manufacturing and maintaining large, roughly shaped unifacial tools, while Late Prehistoric knapping may have emphasized the fine working of small projectile points.
- The contrasts may reflect differences in the lithic material sources that were used. Archaic knappers, living for the most part on coastal mesas and in coastal valleys, may have exploited primarily local cobble sources, with a high ratio of cortex to the volume of usable rock. Late Prehistoric knappers, based farther inland or making greater use of inland resources, may have made more extensive use of material quarried from outcrop sources, such as the Santiago Peak Volcanics.
- The contrasts may reflect differences in settlement organization. If Archaic peoples were near the foraging end of the foraging/collecting continuum (Binford 1980; cf. Laylander 1997b), shifting their habitation bases to be near resources, they may have tended to bring relatively unworked lithic material to those bases for initial reduction. If Late Prehistoric peoples were more logistically organized collectors, they may have done most of the initial, cortex-removing reduction at quarries or special-function workshops.
- In some instances, the contrasts may reflect differences in archaeological recovery techniques, such as the inclusion or exclusion of surface collections or the size of the screen mesh used. Within particular assemblages, smaller pieces of debitage have been found consistently to have lower frequencies of cortex.
PROSPECTS
Future archaeological investigations may be able to determine whether the frequency of cortex on lithic debitage is a usable index of site chronology, or whether differences in this variable are explained by other factors.